The
word “moral” is derived from the words “mores”. Mores refers to customs,
folkways and conventions of a social group. The social group members are
expected to conform to their mores.
Moral
behaviour refers to a behaviour that tends to the good and rejects the evil.
Behaviour is evil if it is unacceptable to and abhorred by a social group.
Developing morality therefore signifies understanding and following a society’s
rights and wrongs.
Moral
development describes changes in children’s sense of fairness, of what is right
and what is wrong. Note that the child’s behaviour in this context is compared
to the moral standard of the social group.
At
the end of this page, you should be able to:
· Describe
the nature of morality
· Outline
the sequence in the development of moral judgement
· List
and discuss the major factors that influence moral development
· Discuss
the educational significance of moral development.
The
Nature of Morality
It
is difficult to give a precise definition of the nature of morality. Different
perspectives define morality differently.
According
to Emmanuel Kant, a moral act is an act done from duty. An act done from duty
differs from an act done in accordance with duty.
An
act is done from duty when the person acting does so because they feel some
sense of obligation. Indeed, the individual has the option, and may choose not
to act that way.
Act
done in accordance with duty carries with it some sense of compulsion. There is
fear of punishment. If we define a moral act as an act done from duty or from a
sense of obligation, it follows that a moral act is an act that can be applied
to anybody. For example, the proposition “I help my neighbour in distress
because I help anybody in distress” qualifies to be a moral act. It can be
applied to anybody including oneself. In this sense, morality is a universal
principle guiding human conduct.
Morality
is conduct or behaviour arising
from some internalized standard without
reference to any group’s standard of behaviour, or to some possible consequence to the individual. A moral behaviour
is not selfish. It is not prudential. It is not random.
Sociologists,
however, define morality in relative terms. They conceive morality from the
viewpoint of a reference group
standard. To them, an act is moral, if it conforms to the particular
reference group standard.
This
is, group mores, customs or expectations.
Therefore,
sociologists define morality as behaviour that respects the rules and
institutions of a society. They lay emphasis on obedience to rules and
regulations. To sociologists, there is no moral act that can be applied to
anybody. There are no moral universals.
Developmental
psychologists consider morality in terms of children’s reasoning when faced
with moral issues, and their
attitude to moral transgressions. To them, morality is
the ability to discriminate between
right and wrong.
Development of Moral Judgment
It
is amazing to consider the great capacity of human beings have for good and
evil. The question: “where does morality come from?” has often been posed.
We
consider here two perspectives on the development of moral judgement, namely:
the perspectives of the social learning theorists and the cognitive-developmental
theorists.
The
Social Learning Perspective
Social
learning theorists focus on how prosocial
behaviour evolves. Prosocial behaviour is a helping behaviour that is directed to benefit another person.
Social learning theory emerges in children as a result of their interaction with people in their
immediate and wider environment.
The
key issue in social learning relates to how rewards and punishments have been managed to engender morally
appropriate behaviour in children.
The children are members of society. Therefore, they are expected to have regard to approvals and disapprovals
or generalized reinforcers of
society. It is by anticipating such generalised reinforcers that the child exerts self-control
over their behaviour. Therefore, morality
originates from reinforcements provided
by significant others in
society.
Observation
of models play a significant
role in children’s learning of prosocial behaviour. Abstract modelling explains how this learning occurs. In
abstract modelling, the child identifies with the model. Thus, when the model
is directly rewarded for a pro-social behaviour, the child is indirectly
rewarded also.
Abstract
modelling, therefore is the process in which modelling paves the way for the
development of more general rules and principles of behaviour. What this means
is that, not all pro-social behaviours have to be emitted and rewarded directly
for watch child for the child to learn general rules.
Observing
a model receive reinforcement is adequate indirect reinforcement for learning
to occur.
The Cognitive-Developmental Perspective
This
will be discussed under the following sub-topics.
Piaget’s
Cognitive-Developmental Perspective
Piaget
(1994) reasoned that moral development follows a developmental pattern indicating increasing understanding of the
meaning of justice.
Moral
development, therefore, may be conceived as unfolding through three main
levels:
Level
One – Heteronemous Morality
Children
aged 4 to 7 years are found operating under the heteronomous morality level. Children at this level believe in imminent justice.
Misconducts
should be punished immediately. Rules are seen as unchangeable, and never
varying. The rules have been created by an authority.
Level
Two – Incipient Cooperation
Children
7 – 10 years fall in this level of morality. Play for children at this level
becomes a clear social activity. They are capable of learning formal rules, and play their games
according to this shared knowledge.
Rules
are still seen as unchangeable though they understand the rules.
Level
Three – Autonomous Cooperation
Children
10 years plus are found in the autonomous
level of morality.
Children
at this level become aware that formal game rules may be modified by those who
play it. They now know that rules of law are created by human beings.
Therefore,
rules are subject to change as the players may wish. Issue of rules and reasoning on justice are no longer
bounded in the concrete.
Intentions
are now taken into account in matters of justice and morality.
Kohlberg’s
Cognitive-Developmental Perspective
Kohlberg
(1994) contends that children pass through a series of stage in evolving a
sense of justice and reasoning on moral issues.
To
him, the evolution of moral judgement is tied to cognitive development. For
example, school age children think either in terms of concrete, unwavering
rules or in terms of the rules of society.
By
adolescence, however, they are capable of reasoning on a higher plane, having
attained formal level cognitive capacity. Adolescents are able to comprehend abstract principles of morality. Their
standard for judging moral issues become predicated on conscience.
Kohlberg
suggested a three-level sequence for understanding the development of moral
judgement:
Level
One – Pre-conventional Morality
At
this level, children follow unwavering rules based on rewards and punishments.
What inspires moral judgement is related to obedience, and self satisfaction.
Level
Two – Conventional Morality
At
the level of conventional morality, children approach moral problems in terms
of their own position as good,
responsible members of society.
This
is what Kohlberg called: Good boy,
good girl orientation.
Level
Three – Post-Conventional Morality
At
this level, children invoke universal
moral principles that are considered broader than the rules of the
particular society children find themselves. To do the right thing is an
obligation. Morality is then considered duty to one’s own conscience based on universal ethics.
Factors that influence Moral Development
Several
factors influence moral development.
Among
them are:
·
Maturation
·
Rules and Regulation
·
Modeling and Rewards.
Maturation
The
cognitive-developmental perspective maintains moral judgement unfolds with
increasing maturation. The quality of a child’s moral reasoning is related to
the quality of cognitive capacity of the child.
Cognitive
development is predicated on level of maturation.
Rules
and Regulations
Rules
and regulations serve as guidelines for prosocial behaviour. They specify
general expectations and thereby serve as a source of motivation to the child
to conform to social expectation. The specific purpose for each rule or
regulation when explained is an invaluable guide to behaviour. It serves as
internalised reasons to conform.
Moral
development of children is not a matter of instruction and preaching.
Significant others must also model the prosocial behaviours that they expect
children to exhibit. Indeed, there is great need that parents and caregivers,
in addition to instruction, should also “walk
the talk” for
the children to emulate.
Rewards
The
child needs a confirmation whenever their behaviour conforms to expectation. Effective
contingency management helps in
engendering prosocial behaviours of children, and also discouraging anti-social
behaviours.
Educational Implications of Moral Development
The
educational implications of moral development are as follows:
·
Morality is not inborn or given by the creator. It is acquired through living
in a community of human relations. There is need for both formal and informal
education (the school and the family) to provide adequate opportunities for
children to observe others acting a cooperative, helpful manner.
·
Children should be encouraged to interact with peers in joint activities in
which they share a common goal.
·
Though society prescribes rules, regulations and standards for conduct,
children should not be made to harbour a feeling of vileness or guilt for minor
infractions on the standards of conduct. For one thing, children’s
understanding and interpretations of standards of conduct is at best,
relatively naĂŻve.
·
Education in morality should not emphasise the intrinsic rightness or wrongness
of actions. Children should be led to see that there are always alternative
explanations for others’ behaviour. Children should be made to understand that
the behaviour of their peers has several possible interpretations.
·
While a child’s level of maturation may limit their understanding of moral
precepts, the school should not just sit back and wait for maturation to occur.
Intellectual discourse on moral issues may speed up the appearance of relevant
cognitive structures that permit the child to reason about morality. Therefore,
the school should provide a good setting in which values; beliefs and opinions
may be critically examined.
·
It may be true that contingency management engender prosocial behaviour,
however, parents and teachers should be wary in the use of punishments or
reproach to instill moral behaviour. The value of punishment in changing
behaviour is very uncertain.
Parents
and teachers should rather select the appropriate behaviours exhibited by the
child and nurture these through a system of rewards.
·
Parents and teachers should make effort to explicitly teach children moral
reasoning and self-control. Rules and regulations should be explained and
understood in terms of their value for all stakeholders in a community of relations.
Rules and regulations are no absolutes designed to tame children’s freedom or
excesses.
Children
are neither good nor bad by their nature. Moral behaviour is learned, like most
other behaviours of children. The capacity for moral judgement and moral
decision unfolds with maturation and experience.
There
are universal principles that guide moral behaviour. Your duty, as a caregiver,
is to create an enabling environment to challenge children to reason about
moral issues. It is not enough to preach moral dogmas; adults must also “walk
the talk”. Example is the best moral precept.
0 Comments